Organizational and executive function problems in ADHD - neurophysiological correlates
Organizational problems are an essential part of executive functions.
A description of the executive functions can be found at ⇒ Organizational difficulties / executive problems In the article ⇒ Complete list of ADHD symptoms according to manifestations in the chapter ⇒ Symptoms.
Executive functions are primarily controlled by working memory.
ADHD is characterized by 3 pathways (according to Sonuga-Barke) that cause neurophysiological symptoms:
-
Dopamine deficiency (among others) in dlPFC (working memory)
- Executive functions
- Disorganization
- Forgetfulness
- Executive functions
-
Dopamine deficiency (among others) in the striatum (reinforcement center)
- Motivation problems
- Impulsiveness
- Hyperactivity
- Changes in the cerebellum
- Time perception problems
- 1. The working memory
- 2. Working memory problems with ADHD
- 3. Measurement of working memory problems
- 4. Other executive functions
- 5. Cerebellum damage as a cause of executive problems
- 6. Insufficient use of neuronal resources
1. The working memory
Working memory supports the ability to temporarily store and manipulate information in order to control goal-oriented behavior.
Goldman-Rakic defined working memory as the ability to recall events in the absence of direct stimulation.1 For this purpose, reciprocal feed-forward inhibition between groups of pyramidal neurons is used.
The working memory consists of three memory units that are coordinated by a central instance:
- Processing processor unit (“central executive”)
- DlPFC
- Processes information that is stored in the two memory stores
- Visual-spatial memory
- Right posterior parietal lobe
- Acoustic-linguistic (“phonological”) memory
- Lower left parietal lobe
- Short-term memory
- Episodic buffer for visual-spatial and acoustic-linguistic information for processing by the central executive. Memory capacity on average 7 (+/-2) units (Miller’s number)
Transferred to a PC, the central executive could be described as the main processor, the short-term memory as the working memory (RAM) and the memory storage as different segments of the hard disk.
Working memory in the dlPFC is primarily dopaminergic and noradrenergic.
Both dopamine and noradrenaline influence working memory in an inverted-U form. Too little dopamine or noradrenaline impair the working memory just as much as too much dopamine or noradrenaline.2
1.1. Working memory and dopamine
The processes of working memory appear to be controlled predominantly by phasic rather than tonic dopamine.3 For example, DA neurons react phasically to stimuli that are to be memorized and show no tonic activity during the retention interval, i.e. the time during which the information is kept active.4
D1 agonists improve, D1 antagonists impair the function of working memory.5 Excessive D1 stimulation, such as during acute stress, leads to working memory deficits, as does insufficient D1 stimulation.678
D2 receptor agonists improve, D2 antagonists impair spatial (not non-spatial) working memory and thus executive functions in healthy adults.9
1.2. Working memory and noradrenaline
Working memory is also noradrenergically controlled in humans and rats, for example.101112
Activation of postsynaptic α1-adrenoceptors by stress impairs working memory via activation of the phosphatidylinositol protein kinase C (PKC) pathway.13 Guanfacine and clonidine improved spatial working memory in aging or younger PFC-lesioned monkeys.14
The amount of noradrenaline released in the PFC determines which type of adrenoreceptor is addressed. Moderate levels of noradrenaline activate high-affinity a-2A receptors (Gi coupled, cAMP inhibitory), while higher levels of noradrenaline, as released during stress, activate low-affinity a-1 receptors (phosphotidyl inositol coupled) and low-affinity b-1 receptors, which increases cAMP signaling. Moderate levels of noradrenaline improve working memory in the PFC via the high-affinity a-2A adrenoreceptor, whereas stimulation of the D1 receptor, a-1 adrenoreceptor and b-1 adrenoreceptor (low-affinity for noradrenaline) impairs working memory.58
1.3. Working memory and CRH
CRH in general and in PFC in particular impairs visual-spatial working memory in a dose-dependent manner, which is particularly impaired in ADHD.15 Non-specific CRH receptor antagonists such as selective CRH-1 receptor antagonists remedied the impairment of working memory and were therefore considered by the authors as possible starting points for treatment of ADHD.
2. Working memory problems with ADHD
ADHD correlates with working memory problems,16 unlike tic disorders.17
The various components of working memory in ADHD appear to be impaired with varying degrees of frequency (and very differently from individual to individual). Around 70% of those affected showed an impairment in one of the 3 areas:18
- Reorganization of the working memory (very common)
- Retaining and rearranging information
- Updating the working memory (frequent)
- Actively monitoring incoming information and replacing outdated information with relevant information
- 8% of those affected showed particular strength in this area
- Dual processing (rare)
- Maintaining information while performing a secondary task
- 20% of those affected showed particular strength in this area
In the dlPFC, sustained attention to solving problems is mapped. The ADHD symptoms caused by impaired sustained attention therefore correlate with a disorder of the dlPFC.19 Selective attention (distractibility), on the other hand, is located in the dorsal nucleus accumbens.
Working memory problems in ADHD do not diminish in adulthood. In some cases, a deterioration in distractibility has been observed.20
Another study surprisingly found that language acquisition and arithmetic math skills, which require working memory, were not significantly impaired in children with ADHD. However, performance dropped significantly when those affected believed they were less gifted.21
In addition to executive problems, one study also found problems with the Theory of Mind (ToM) in children with ADHD (in contrast to other studies). However, these did not correlate with the executive problems, so that an involvement of the working memory seemed unlikely.22 Another study found reduced Theory of Mind abilities in adults with ADHD, which correlated with executive problems.23
In children with ADHD, a correlation was found between impaired working memory and eye movement abnormalities during reading. Visual scanning of words during reading was discontinuous, uncoordinated and chaotic. ADHD groups showed higher entropy index among the four categories of saccades than non-affected groups.16
With ADHD, it takes 250 ms for an error in sentence structure to be detected. For non-affected people, it only takes 100 ms.24
Organizational difficulties (disorganization) are often associated with time perception problems.25 However, this is not consistent with the neurophysiological correlates of organizational problems / executive problems described here (deficits in working memory, which is located in the dlPFC), while the neurophysiological correlates of time (perception) problems are primarily located in the cerebellum.
One study found evidence that the cognitive deficits of patients with ADHD that can be measured with the N-back task are not due to a deficit in working memory, but to a disturbance of the cognitive state (memory load, task duration and new stimuli). ADHD patients and controls showed no significant differences in terms of reaction time and accuracy:26
- Spatially, adult ADHD patients showed significantly higher activation levels of oxyHb in the left orbitofrontal area and left frontopolar area (channels 4 and 11) in the 2-back task and lower activation levels of deoxyHb in the 3-back task than healthy controls (corrected p < 0.05).
- In terms of time, adults with ADHD reached their peak ROIs earlier than healthy controls.
3. Measurement of working memory problems
The problem-solving skills and sustained attention items associated with working memory can be measured with tests.
3.1. Measuring working memory problems with the N-back test
Working memory and sustained attention can be tested with the N-back test.27
Find out more at ⇒ N-back test In the subsection ⇒ Attention and reaction tests in the section ⇒ Tests in the article ⇒ ADHD - diagnostic methods in the chapter ⇒ Diagnostics.
4. Other executive functions
Inhibition (control of reaction inhibition)28
Error detection
Control of interference (a component of reaction inhibition)
Planning and solving problems28
correct execution of working memory tasks28
- DlPFC
- VlPFC
- Rostral PFC
- Parietal cortex (bilateral and medial-posterior)
Word fluency:28
-
Phonemic word fluency
Number of words with a certain initial letter that a test band can form:- Frontal regions33
- Left motor/premotor regions
- Left or bilateral opercular regions
- Left lateral orbitofrontal regions
- Right dorsolateral regions
- Frontal regions33
-
Semantic / categorical word fluency
Number of words that a respondent can form from a certain semantic category (e.g. animals), regardless of the initial letter- Temporal regions33
cognitive flexibility28
- Cortex inferior parietal
- Superior colliculus
- Posterior lateral thalamus
- Medial frontal regions
- Pre-supplementary motor area
5. Cerebellum damage as a cause of executive problems
Children with tumor-related damage to the cerebellum showed significantly more frequent executive problems and social-emotional problems.34
6. Insufficient use of neuronal resources
One study investigated the access to brain capacities during a more resource-intensive task in patients with ADHD. It was found that executive dysfunction during a more resource-intensive task could be due to insufficient utilization (allocation) of neuronal resources.35 The difference was shown in a reduced pupil dilation.
Goldman-Rakic PS (1995): Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. 1995 Mar;14(3):477-85. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6. PMID: 7695894. ↥
Arnsten AF (2007):. Catecholamine and second messenger influences on prefrontal cortical networks of “representational knowledge”: a rational bridge between genetics and the symptoms of mental illness. Cereb Cortex. 2007 Sep;17 Suppl 1:i6-15. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm033. PMID: 17434919. REVIEW ↥
Cohen, Braver, Brown (2002): Computational perspectives on dopamine function in prefrontal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002 Apr;12(2):223-9. ↥
Hollerman, Tremblay, Schultz (1998): Influence of Reward Expectation on Behavior-Related Neuronal Activity in Primate Striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology 1998 80:2, 947-963 ↥
Arnsten (1997): Catecholamine regulation of the prefrontal cortex. J Psychopharmacol. 1997;11(2):151-62. ↥ ↥
Arnsten (2001): Dopaminergic and noradrenergic influences on cognitive functions mediated by prefrontal cortex. In Solanto, Arnsten, Castellanos (Herausgeber): Stimulant drugs and ADHD: Basic and clinical neuroscience (p. 185–208). Oxford University Press. Zitiert nach Solanto (2002): Dopamine dysfunction in AD/HD: integrating clinical and basic neuroscience research. Behav Brain Res. 2002 Mar 10;130(1-2):65-71. ↥
Arnsten AF, Li BM (2005): Neurobiology of executive functions: catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical functions. Biol Psychiatry. 2005 Jun 1;57(11):1377-84. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.019. PMID: 15950011. REVIEW ↥
Levy F (2009): Dopamine vs noradrenaline: inverted-U effects and ADHD theories. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2009 Feb;43(2):101-8. doi: 10.1080/00048670802607238. PMID: 19153917. REVIEW ↥ ↥
Mehta, Sahakian, Robbins (2001): Comparative psychopharmacology of methylphenidate and related drugs in human volunteers, patients with ADHD, and experimental animals. In Solanto, Arnsten, Castellanos (Herausgeber): Stimulant drugs and ADHD: Basic and clinical neuroscience (p. 303–331). Oxford University Press. Zitiert nach Solanto (2002): Dopamine dysfunction in AD/HD: integrating clinical and basic neuroscience research. Behav Brain Res. 2002 Mar 10;130(1-2):65-71. ↥
Arnsten AF (2009): Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009 Jun;10(6):410-22. doi: 10.1038/nrn2648. PMID: 19455173; PMCID: PMC2907136. REVIEW ↥
Chamberlain SR, Robbins TW (2013): Noradrenergic modulation of cognition: therapeutic implications. J Psychopharmacol. 2013 Aug;27(8):694-718. doi: 10.1177/0269881113480988. Erratum in: J Psychopharmacol. 2013 Oct;27(10):964. PMID: 23518815. REVIEW ↥
Robbins TW, Arnsten AF (2009): The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: monoaminergic modulation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:267-87. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135535. PMID: 19555290; PMCID: PMC2863127. REVIEW ↥
Birnbaum SG, Yuan PX, Wang M, Vijayraghavan S, Bloom AK, Davis DJ, Gobeske KT, Sweatt JD, Manji HK, Arnsten AF (2004): Protein kinase C overactivity impairs prefrontal cortical regulation of working memory. Science. 2004 Oct 29;306(5697):882-4. doi: 10.1126/science.1100021. PMID: 15514161. ↥
Holland N, Robbins TW, Rowe JB (2021): The role of noradrenaline in cognition and cognitive disorders. Brain. 2021 Sep 4;144(8):2243-2256. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab111. PMID: 33725122; PMCID: PMC8418349. REVIEW ↥
Hupalo, Berridge (2016): Working Memory Impairing Actions of Corticotropin-Releasing Factor (CRF) Neurotransmission in the Prefrontal Cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016 Oct;41(11):2733-40. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.85. PMID: 27272767; PMCID: PMC5026742. ↥
Mohammadhasani, Caprì, Nucita, Iannizzotto, Fabio (2019): Atypical Visual Scan Path Affects Remembering in ADHD. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2019 Dec 12:1-10. doi: 10.1017/S135561771900136X. ↥ ↥
Openneer, Forde, Akkermans, Naaijen, Buitelaar, Hoekstra, Dietrich (2019): Executive function in children with Tourette syndrome and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Cross-disorder or unique impairments? Cortex. 2019 Dec 3;124:176-187. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.11.007. ↥
Fosco, Kofler, Groves, Chan, Raiker (2020): Which ‘Working’ Components of Working Memory aren’t Working in Youth with ADHD? J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020 Jan 27;10.1007/s10802-020-00621-y. doi: 10.1007/s10802-020-00621-y. PMID: 31989344. n = 86 ↥
Stahl (2013): Stahl’s Essential Psychopharmacology, 4. Auflage, Chapter 12: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and its treatment, Seite 472 ↥
Torgalsbøen, Zeiner, Øie (2019): Pre-attention and Working Memory in ADHD: A 25-Year Follow-Up Study. J Atten Disord. 2019 Oct 18:1087054719879491. doi: 10.1177/1087054719879491. ↥
Turker, Seither-Preisler, Reiterer, Schneider (2019): Cognitive and Behavioural Weaknesses in Children with Reading Disorder and AD(H)D. Sci Rep. 2019 Oct 23;9(1):15185. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-51372-w. ↥
Mohammadzadeh, Khorrami Banaraki, Tehrani Doost, Castelli (2019): A new semi-nonverbal task glance, moderate role of cognitive flexibility in ADHD children’s theory of mind. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2019 Oct 29:1-17. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2019.1681951. ↥
Tatar, Cansız (2020): Executive function deficits contribute to poor theory of mind abilities in adults with ADHD. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2020 Mar 18:1-8. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2020.1736074. PMID: 32186409. n = 80 ↥
Gonzalez-Perez, Hernandez-Exposito, Perez, Ramirez, Dominguez (2018): Electrophysiological correlates of reading in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Rev Neurol. 2018 Mar 16;66(6):175-181. n = 79 ↥
Li Y, Chen J, Zheng X, Liu J, Peng C, Liao Y, Liu Y. (2022): Cognitive deficit in adults with ADHD lies in the cognitive state disorder rather than the working memory deficit: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. J Psychiatr Res. 2022 Aug 1;154:332-340. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.07.064. PMID: 36029728. n = 46 ↥
Stahl (2013): Stahl’s Essential Psychopharmacology, 4. Auflage, Chapter 12: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and its treatment, Seite 473 ↥
Pitzianti, Spiridigliozzi, Bartolucci, Esposito, Pasini (2020): New Insights on the Effects of Methylphenidate in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2020 Sep 30;11:531092. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.531092. PMID: 33132928; PMCID: PMC7561436. ↥ ↥ ↥ ↥ ↥ ↥
Rubia, Smith, Brammer, Taylor (2003): Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection. Neuroimage. 2003 Sep;20(1):351-8. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00275-1. PMID: 14527595. ↥ ↥
Aron, Poldrack (2006): Cortical and subcortical contributions to Stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci. 2006 Mar 1;26(9):2424-33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006. PMID: 16510720; PMCID: PMC6793670. ↥
Lie, Specht, Marshall, Fink (2006): Using fMRI to decompose the neural processes underlying the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Neuroimage. 2006 Apr 15;30(3):1038-49. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.031. PMID: 16414280. ↥ ↥ ↥ ↥ ↥
Peterson, Skudlarski, Gatenby, Zhang, Anderson, Gore (1999): An fMRI study of Stroop word-color interference: evidence for cingulate subregions subserving multiple distributed attentional systems. Biol Psychiatry. 1999 May 15;45(10):1237-58. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00056-6. PMID: 10349031. ↥ ↥
Baldo, Schwartz, Wilkins, Dronkers (2006): Role of frontal versus temporal cortex in verbal fluency as revealed by voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006 Nov;12(6):896-900. doi: 10.1017/S1355617706061078. PMID: 17064451. ↥ ↥
Starowicz-Filip A, Bętkowska-Korpała B, Yablonska T, Kwiatkowski S, Milczarek O, Klasa Ł, Chrobak AA (2022): Involvement of the cerebellum in the regulation of executive functions in children-Preliminary analysis based on a neuropsychological study of children after cerebellar tumour surgery. Front Psychol. 2022 Oct 6;13:961577. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.961577. PMID: 36275206; PMCID: PMC9583864. ↥
Chung J, Lee P, Lee YB, Yoo K, Jeong Y (2022): Nonuniformity of Whole-Cerebral Neural Resource Allocation, a Neuromarker of the Broad-Task Attention. eNeuro. 2022 Mar 14;9(2):ENEURO.0358-21.2022. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0358-21.2022. PMID: 35228309; PMCID: PMC8925723. ↥